About four years ago I went under the knife
for a procedure many teenagers will endure; wisdom teeth extraction. I’d heard
a lot of horror stories about what it might be like – stories about dentists
who need to stand up on the chair to get leverage, for instance; gruesome descriptions
of the cracking noise that comes as teeth are wrenched from their sockets
one-by-one; etc. etc.
My actual experience, however, was somewhat
enjoyable. Plainly, I don’t remember much. What I do remember is a cold feeling
running up my arm (after an injection of general anesthetic in my wrist), and
waking up (seemingly only a moment later) feeling awfully cheerful and relaxed,
and dribbling uncontrollably. ‘Wow!’ I thought, ‘That’s what a good night’s
sleep should feel like!’
You might not have had a general anesthetic
before, but I bet you’ve tried alcohol (or other substances) and experienced a shift
in your consciousness. Drugs work. But why? The insight my wisdom-teeth
experience afforded me, and which David Eagleman expounds upon in his new book Incognito:
The Secret Lives of the Brain, is that thoughts
are physical.
Eagleman is a neuroscientist at the Baylor
College of Medicine, where he directs the Laboratory for Perception and Action,
as well as the Centre for Neuroscience and Law. Incognito is his elegant attempt to lampoon the dualistic view
(that the mind and body are separate) that has dominated western thought for
centuries, and to demonstrate how truly bizarre the three-pound bag of goop
inside your skull is in its operations.
Consider this: there are a disproportionate
number of people named Larry and Laura (and other names starting with L) in the
legal profession. Similarly, the average person is more likely to marry someone
with a name beginning with the same first letter as his or her own. But if you ask
Leonard why he married Leanne and decided to go into the landscaping business,
don’t expect him to own up to the powerful influence of the alphabet.
Some more surprises: men consider women far
more attractive just before they ovulate. No one knows why, exactly, but it
could have something to do with pheromones – chemical messages secreted by the
body that signal fertility. The effect is pronounced: one study in New Mexico
found that female strippers raked in an average of $68 per hour during peak
fertility, compared to a normal average of $52 (and only $35 during ovulation).
What these examples, and dozens more in
Eagleman’s book highlight, is that the conscious aspect of our cognition is by
no means the most central to our actions and choices. Although he lacked the
tools that we have today to get inside the head, it seems that Freud – despite
some questionable forays into unfalsifiable theory creation (e.g. the Oedipus complex) – was essentially right
to believe in the paramountcy of the unconscious.
Incognito offers an enjoyable amble through social
psychology and modern neuroscience, but many will have heard about much of this
material (about our non-rational side etc.) before. The most intriguing
insights – at least for me – come, instead, in the form of Eagleman’s musings on
neuroscience and the law; principally, about what a deeper understanding of the
biological bases for our actions means for concepts like culpability and punishment.
In 1966, a 25 year-old engineering student
at the University of Texas, Charles Whitman, opened fire on his fellow students
from the heights of the main building’s tower. Only shortly before, Whitman –
who had an IQ of 138, and had been previously known as an intelligent and
friendly young man – had killed his mother and wife (stabbing the latter while
she slept). As the nation grieved, many asked a simple question: ‘Why?’
A note Whitman had composed the night
before, offered some answers:
I do not really
understand myself these days. I am supposed to be an average reasonable and
intelligent young man. However, lately (I cannot recall when it started) I have
been a victim of many unusual and irrational thoughts.
Also in his note, Whitman asked that an
autopsy be performed after his death to determine if something had changed in
his brain. And in an eerie harbinger of what was to come, he asked that his
money be donated anonymously to a mental health foundation; “Maybe research can
prevent further tragedies of this type.” Whitman’s intuition was spot-on: an
autopsy, performed shortly after he was killed at the scene, revealed an
aggressive brain tumour known as a glioblastoma,
which had compressed his amygdala – a
region of the brain associated with emotional regulation.
What can cases like Whitman’s tell us about
our criminal justice system? At first glance, not much – our system already
enables such people to get off the hook, if it can be shown that their actions
were not, for instance, voluntary. But if neuroscience is beginning to explain
how all of our behavior is caused by
our biological states – and that, consequently, we have no meaningful ‘free
will’ (because such states are influenced by factors beyond our control) –
shouldn’t we be letting everyone walk free?
Eagleman outlines an evidence-based
approach to criminal justice that is forward-looking, and which acknowledges
the incoherency of retributivism:
While our current
style of punishment rests on a bedrock of personal volition and blame, the
present line of argument suggests an alternative. Although societies possess
deeply ingrained impulses for punishment, a forward-looking legal system would
be more concerned with how to best serve the society from this day forward …
Prison terms do not have to be based on a desire for bloodlust, but instead can
be calibrated to the risk of reoffending.
Of course, that is only the beginning of a
rich conversation. But it suffices here for me to say that I think Eagleman’s
approach is persuasive. If we take it seriously, it could require no less than
a fundamental restructure of our criminal justice system. Such blue-sky
thinking, besides the elegant descriptions of our brain’s ins-and-outs, makes Incognito a worthwhile read.
An
interesting presentation by David Eagleman on ‘The Brain and the Law’ can be
found here.
--
William Isdale is a law and arts (politics and philosophy) student at the University of Queensland, where he is an Academic Excellence Scholar and TJ Ryan Medallist and Scholar. He is the President of the Australian Legal Philosophy Students' Association and Editor of the Justice and the Law Society's journal 'Pandora's Box'. In early 2012 he was a visiting student at Oxford University's Uehiro Centre for Practical Ethics.